As we have seen, the effects of the accent shift
accompanying the formation of Indo-European collectives were levelled out in
Greek and Vedic. Note that such analogical regularisation happens when speakers
find it difficult to make sense of the forms they are exposed to. Ancient
alternations lose their productivity and become obscured by accumulated layers
of sound change. If the outcome survives, it lingers on as a grammatical
irregularity. If the whole speech community gets rid of it, the evidence that could
be used to reconstruct the original alternation is lost. Fortunately for
historical linguists, speakers are not very consistent in “repairing” the
irregularities of their language. For example, in the prehistory of Greek the
accent and the vocalism of the singular and the collective of ‘wheel’ were
levelled out. Nevertheless, speakers didn’t mess with the inherited gender of
the word: kúklos remained a masculine despite having a neuter-like plural. We
can imagine that a different language could change the gender of the word but
preserve clear traces of the accent alternation. And that indeed is what
Germanic has done.
The
best-know features distinguishing the Germanic languages from the rest of
Indo-European are the consequences of two regular sound changes which operated
in the common ancestor of the group (Proto-Germanic): Grimm’s Law and Verner’s
Law. Grimm’s Law affected all the inherited Indo-European stops,
changing their phonation type (voicing) or manner of articulation. The
pre-Germanic voiceless stops *p, *t, *k, *kʷ became voiceless fricatives with
the same or similar place of articulation: *f, *þ, *x, *xʷ. At roughly the same
time the inherited “voiced aspirated” stops *bʰ, *dʰ, *gʰ, *gʷʰ shifted into corresponding voiced fricatives: *β, *ð, *ɣ, *ɣʷ. A little later, the third part of Grimm’s Law was enacted: the remaining inherited stops *b, *d,
*g, *gʷ became devoiced, yielding Germanic *p, *t, *k, *kʷ. As a result, Proto-Germanic
changed from a language with a large number of stop phonemes into a language
with a rich system of fricatives.
Verner’s Law applied to non-initial voiceless fricatives not
adjacent to another voiceless sound and preceded by an unaccented syllable. The fricatives affected were either
those generated by Grimm’s Law, or the only fricative phoneme inherited from pre-Germanic times, *s (the Proto-Indo-European “laryngeal” fricatives had already disappeared). As a
result, *f, *þ, *s, *x, *xʷ became *β, *ð, *z, *ɣ, *ɣʷ in the appropriate environment.
|
From *kʷékʷlos to wheel? It wasn’t that simple. |
Let us see
how these changes affected PIE *kʷékʷlos.
Grimm’s Law applied to both occurrences of *kʷ, changing
them into *xʷ. Since neither of them was found in an environment triggering
Verner’s Law, they remained unchanged till the end of Proto-Germanic. Verner’s
Law would have affected the final *-s. but we can’t be sure it was there. The
‘wheel’ word is a neuter noun in Northwest Germanic. We don’t know if it survived in
Gothic, the only East Germanic language known from written texts. Most of the
preserved Gothic material consists of copies of one partial translation of the
Bible, and the text doesn’t happen to mention wheels. It’s obvious that the
shift from masculine to neuter in the singular noun took place because the
plural looked neuter, but we can’t tell whether it happened in pre-Germanic,
Proto-Germanic or the common ancestor of the Northwest Germanic languages.
Let us therefore ignore the gendered nom.sg. ending *-s and focus on the stem *kʷékʷlo-, which was the same for neuters and masculines. In the
passage from pre-Germanic to Proto-Germanic, *kʷékʷlo- > *xʷéxʷla-.[1]
What happened to the the collective *kʷəkʷláh₂? The
laryngeal in the ending was lost long before Grimm’s Law, and the
vowel was lengthened by compensation. It seems that a full vowel was restored early in the initial syllable on the analogy of the singular, so we may start with the form *kʷekʷlā́,
serving as the plural of *kʷékʷlo- (whatever the latter’s gender). In
Proto-Germanic, *kʷekʷlā́ became *xʷexʷlṓ by Grimm’s Law. Since the second *xʷ
occurred in a voiced environment after an unaccented vowel, Verner’s Law
applied, yielding *xʷeɣʷlṓ as the plural of *xʷéxʷla-.
A few more
developments took place before the split of Proto-Germanic into the East and Northwest groups.
First, Proto-Germanic gave up contrastive accent in favour of fixed initial
stress. This means that although linguists can sometimes infer the original location of the word accent from the outcome of Verner’s Law, both *xʷexʷla- and *xʷeɣʷlō had predictable initial stress in late
Proto-Germanic, and the speakers of the language had no means of guessing where
the *xʷ/*ɣʷ alternation came from. The occurrence of “non-Vernerian” and
“Vernerian” variants no longer depended on stress-related factors. For historical reasons, they
were found tendentially in different grammatical forms, so speakers came to regard the
conditioning as morphological, not phonological. But since grammatical
contrasts are in most cases sufficiently signalled by other means (e.g. the use
of inflectional endings), the cost of maintaining an obscure consonantal
alternation may outweigh its functional importance.
Although
the Germanic languages entered the historical scene rather late (in comparison
with Hittite, Greek, Vedic or even Latin), they preserved some remarkably
conservative features. The accent shift distinguishing some singular thematic
nouns (with stems ending in *-o-) from their plurals (original collectives) was
one of them. But the establishment of an initial-stress rule sounded the death
knell of the distinction. The voicing alternation in words containing medial
fricatives was not enough to keep it alive. Speakers of Late Proto-Germanic
eliminated most of the Vernerian alternations from the noun system,
generalising one of the variants at the expense of the other. In the
comparative material we can see only some scattered fossils instead of a
productive pattern.
Levelling
out could happen either way. Some speakers generalised the consonant of the
singular (*xʷexʷla-/*xʷexʷlō ), and others that of the plural (*xʷeɣʷla-/*xʷeɣʷlō). In the Proto-Germanic speech community the basic form of the noun was
effectively duplicated: it could be either *xʷexʷla- or *xʷeɣʷla-, both meaning
‘wheel’ and both occurring with the same case-endings.
Still
before the breakup of Proto-Germanic, the status of labiovelar consonants
became precarious. The voiced labiovelar fricative *ɣʷ was eliminated from most
positions; word-medially it merged with the semivowel *w. Voiceless*xʷ lost its
labial accompaniment (lip-rounding) before consonants. The result was like
this: *xʷexla- ~ *xʷewla-. The correspondence between these variants was
anomalous, since the normal “Vernerian” counterpart of non-labialised *x was *ɣ, not *w. This must have caused
occasional transmission errors: *xʷewla- could be misheard and misinterpreted as *xʷeɣla- (by listeners who anticipated the voiced counterpart of *x). Thus, by the end of the Proto-Germanic period
three variants of the stem were in circulation: *xʷexla- ~ *xʷewla- ~ *xʷeɣla-.
The
resolution of a conflict between competing synonyms doesn’t happen overnight.
In fact, it can take centuries unless speakers have a good reason to prefer one
of the forms. In the case we are discussing, however, none of the competitors had a decisive
advantage over the others, so their evolution proceeded in a “neutral” fashion. If there is no systematic
bias, the relative frequencies of variants will vary randomly until
the least lucky one drops out of use and is seen no more. But the three forms
survived into the languages descended from Proto-Northwest Germanic before any of them reached fixation in the
speech community. To be precise, we can identify reflexes of *xʷexla- and *xʷewla-
in North Germanic, while all three can be found in West Germanic.
Let us now adjust
the notations slightly to catch up with the phonological developments in the West
Germanic languages. In their parent language, the articulation of *x was
weakened in most positions, so that a glottal
aspirate [h] beacame its default pronunciation, with velar or palatal fricatives remaining as positional variants determined by the context. I will therefore use the transcription
*h rather than *x for this evolvoing phoneme. At that stage the surviving labiovelars were treated not as single phonemes but as sequences of
two segments, *hw and *kw (*gw was at best rare; it may have become *g by that time).
We can thus assume
the existence of three variants in Early Proto-West Germanic: *hwehla-, *hwewla-, and *hweɣla-. In West
Germanic there was a tendency for consonants to undergo gemination (or, in plain
English, doubling) when followed by *j or by one of the liquid consonants, *r or
*l. Before *j the doubling was regular and affected all consonants except *z
and *r (which soon merged as *r). Before *r and *l, it
was sporadic and restricted to non-sonorants. Some speakers doubled the second *h of *hwehla-, so a new pronunciation,*hwehhla-, was added to the already
existing pool of variants.[2] At a later
stage of Proto-West Germanic the stem dropped its final vowel in the nominative/accusative
singular. The four variants competing at that time were as follows: *hwehl,
*hwehhl, *hwewl, and *hweɣl.
Soon
afterwards, the West Germanic-speaking Angles, Saxons and Jutes embarked on their
conquest of Britain. A few centuries later Old English began to be
written down regularly. Which ancestral forms survived into literary Old
English? The answer is quite surprising: none had been eliminated. Descendants of all the West Germanic variants
can be identified in the Old English corpus:
- hwēol ~ hwīol < *hwehl and *hwewl (from both sources)
- hweohhol (hweohl- when inflected) < *hwehhl
- hweowol ~ hweowul ~ hweowel (hweowl-) < *hwewl
- hweogul ~ hweogel (hweogl-)[3] < *hweɣl
To be sure,
their relative frequency was non-uniform; hwēol was by far the most common form, followed
by hweow(V)l (which was about half as frequent), with the others lagging behind;
but the competition was by no means over yet.
In Middle
English times (11th-15th c.) this variety was drastically reduced. The
variant whẹ̄l /hweːl/ (from OE hwēol) increased its frequency at the expense of all
alternative forms, ousting them almost completely.[4] The variants whewel,
wheghel, and even whefyl (apparently with with /f/ from /x/, as in laughter an enough) lingered on for
some time, but remained vanishingly rare and dialectally restricted. Their
last remaining traces can be found in proper names, for example in the surname
Whewell. You must have heard
of William Whewell (1794–1866), the scholar who coined the words scientist and
physicist, but you probably didn’t know his name was a fancy variant of wheel.
As you can
well imagine, similarly complicated stories could be spun to present the evolution
of the ‘wheel’ word and its variants in other Northwest Germanic languages.
There are numerous interesting problems
that I can’t discuss here for want of space. For example: what happened to Germanic *xʷexʷla-
in High German? where did the odd-looking Old Frisian variants fiāl and t(h)iāl
come from? Well, I have to stop
somewhere. There are other words waiting to be discussed.
[REDUPLICATION: back to the table of contents]
———
[1] The
mergers *o, *a > *a (for short vowels) and *ō, *ā > *ō (for long vowels)
are also characteristically Germanic.
[2] There was
a phonetic difference between medial *-h- and *-hh- surrounded by vowels or
sonorants. The former underwent gradual weakening into a half-voiced glottal
glide [ɦ] and was eventually dropped in the individual histories of the West
Germanic languages, while *-hh- retained
a strong velar articulation [xx] and survived much longer.
[3] OE g was
still a voiced velar fricative, [ɣ], in this context.
[4] It was
spelt in about twenty different ways, which however indicate more or less the
same pronunciation. Rarer dialectal forms with Middle English /iː/ existed as well. The
vowel of Modern English wheel /(h)wiːl/, however, comes from Middle English /eː/ via
the Great Vowel Shift.